Is Gender Identity Actually In The Brain?
Is Gender Identity Actually In The Brain?
Abstract
A common claim for the etiology of gender dysphoria is that the sense of being a man or women is determined by brain structure and function, and that people with gender dysphoria have an opposite sexed brain, caused by abnormalities in prenatal hormone exposure. There is almost no evidence to support this idea, with commonly referenced brain imaging findings being in specific regions of the brain (rather than the brain as a whole), involving participants on puberty blockers or hormonal treatment, and failing to be replicated. Additional evidence in people with masculinized brains raised female is weak, and could be explained by other reasons than gender identity being biological.
Brain Imaging
One common claim in attempting to explain transgenderism is that brain imaging studies find that trans women have "female brains" and trans men have "male brains", and that gender identity is biological. The brain is most definitely sexually dimorphic, and attempts at "debunking" sexual dimorphism of the brain are logically flawed, politically motivated, and tend to misrepresent evidence (Del Giudice 2021a, 2021b). But does gender identity exist biologically? To answer this, I reviewed brain imaging studies on transgender individuals vs controls [Table 1]. Not any of these studies found that the brain of transgender individuals is typical of the opposite sex. Of the 16 studies, only 6 found any pattern of brain volume or activity trending toward the opposite sex, however these findings were in only in 1-2 areas in each study, all failed to be replicated, and in 4 of these studies, participants were on puberty blockers, hormone treatment, or were homosexual (attracted to own biological sex). The remaining studies generally found differences in specific brain regions in both trans men & women compared to cis men & women.
[Table 1]
IFOF = right inferior fronto-occipital tract
PACC = pregenual anterior cingulate cortex
RSMA = right supplementary motor area
RPCG = right posterior cingulate gyrus
RCN = right caudate nucleus
SSC = secondary somatosensory cortex
SG = supramarginal gyrus
LMTG = left middle temporal gyrus
Gender Identity in People With Opposite Gendered Brains
In addition to trans people not showing "oppositely gendered brains", there is little evidence that those with sexually incongruent brain development are more likely to identify as the opposite sex purely due to having a brain typical of the opposite sex. Congenital adrenal hyperplasia (CAH) is a disorder where a person is excessively exposed to prenatal testosterone, and in girls this causes a "masculinized brain" with behaviors, interests, and personalities typical of men. A review of transgender identity in CAH found that while the majority of CAH girls prefer male playmates, toys, and games, only a small minority had significant indication of gender dysphoria, and the vast majority identified and lived as female (Cloudfindings 2021a). While the minority was slightly larger than in the general population, there is a likely possibility that this could be due to psychological alterations found in CAH which are predictive of gender dysphoria (Cloudfindings 2021a).
Meyer-Bahlberg et al. (2005) found that 22% of XY males with genital abnormalities not due to masculinization in the womb or hormonally, which underwent sex reassignment surgery to live as female, later choose to live as female, and this is taken as suggesting it could be due to their male brain. Reiner & Gearhart (2004) found that 8 of 14 subjects reassigned female at birth due to genital abnormalities at some point chose to live as male later on. Four of the patients did so by declaring they were male spontaneously, whereas the remaining four decided to live as male after becoming aware of being a biological male. All except one of the patients raised female had interests, behaviors, and psychological characteristics typical of males. This is taken as evidence that gender identity is biological, however there are many potential reasons why some patients chose to live as male. Simply knowing about being biologically male was enough for four patients to transition in Reiner & Gearhart (2004), and it's likely a significant portion of the cases in Meyer-Bahlberg et al. (2005) did so for the same reason, however it did not report on their reasoning.
Manzouri & Savic (2019) Possible Neurobiological Underpinnings of Homosexuality and Gender Dysphoria
Feusner et al. (2018) Intrinsic network connectivity and own body perception in gender dysphoria
Nota et al. (2017) Brain functional connectivity patterns in children and adolescents with gender dysphoria: Sex-atypical or not?
Burke et al. (2017) Structural connections in the brain in relation to gender identity and sexual orientation
Burke et al. (2016) Male-typical visuospatial functioning in gynephilic girls with gender dysphoria — organizational and activational effects of testosterone
Nota et al. (2017) Brain sexual differentiation and effects of cross-sex hormone therapy in transpeople:A resting-state functional magnetic resonance study
Hahn et al. (2015) Structural Connectivity Networks of Transgender People
Case et al. (2016) Altered White Matter and Sensory Response to Bodily Sensation in Female-to-Male Transgender Individuals
Baldinger-Melich et al. (2020) Sex Matters: A Multivariate Pattern Analysis of Sex- and Gender-Related Neuroanatomical Differences in Cis- and Transgender Individuals Using Structural Magnetic Resonance Imaging
Hoekzema et al. (2015) Regional volumes and spatial volumetric distribution of gray matter in the gender dysphoric brain
Krans et al. (2014) White Matter Microstructure in Transsexuals and Controls Investigated by Diffusion Tensor Imaging
Luders et al. (2009) Regional gray matter variation in male-to-female transsexualism
Del Giudice (2021a) Binary Thinking About the Sex Binary: A Comment on Joel (2021a)
Del Giudice (2021b) Ideological Bias in the Psychology of Sex and Gender
Cloudfindings (2021a) Gender dysphoria is not equivalent to gender nonconformity
Saraswat et al. (2015) Evidence Supporting the Biological Nature of Gender Identity
Meyer-Bahlberg et al. (2005) Gender identity outcome in female-raised 46,XY persons with penile agenesis, cloacal exstrophy of the bladder, or penile ablation
Spizzirri et al. (2018) Grey and white matter volumes either in treatment-naïve or hormone-treated transgender women: a voxel-based morphometry study
Flint et al. (2020) Biological Sex Classification With Structural Mri Data Shows Increased Misclassification In Transgender Woman
Clemens et al. (2021) Replication of Previous Findings? Comparing Gray Matter Volumes in Transgender Individuals with Gender Incongruence and Cisgender Individuals
Fukao et al. (2021) Gray matter volume differences between transgender men and cisgender women: A voxel-based morphometry study
Reiner & Gearhart (2004) Discordant Sexual Identity in Some Genetic Males with Cloacal Exstrophy Assigned to Female Sex at Birth
Coolidge et al. (2002) The Heritability of Gender Identity Disorder in a Child and Adolescent Twin Sample
Cloudfindings (2021b) Meta Analysis of Personality Disorders & Autism In Gender Dysphoria
Comments
Post a Comment